Meta-Analysis Prevalence Pedophilia Hebephilia by Filip Schuster

From BoyWiki
Revision as of 11:57, 29 September 2014 by Etenne (talk | contribs) (→‎Literature)

Filip Schuster

Every fifth boy and man is pedophilic or hebephilic


Abstract

A meta-analysis of all seven relevant phallometric studies reveals that 22% of normal men show greater or equal sexual arousal to child stimuli (individuals up to 13 years old) than to adult stimuli. Combined results of two of these studies reveal male prevalence rates of about 3% for pedophilia (mostly sexually aroused by prepubescents) and about 16% for hebephilia (mostly sexually aroused by pubescents). Details of these studies are described, and implications of the results for sexual science and society are discussed. }}

1. Introduction

Pedophiles are defined in this text as individuals who are primarily sexually aroused by prepubescents (generally 10 years old and younger) and hebephiles as those mostly sexually aroused by pubescents (generally 11 to 14 years old). There is no consensus in sexual science on how to define the words pedophile and hebephile, but in many phallometric studies (e.g. Freund et al., 1991a; Blanchard et al., 2009) and in the recent scientific debate leading up to the "Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Ed." (e.g., Frances et al., 2011), the words pedophile and hebephile were often defined and used in this or a similar way. A pedophile or a hebephile who does not act on his age preference does not transform himself into an ephebophile or teleiophile. In this text an ephebophile is defined as someone mostly sexually aroused by 15- to 17-year-olds, and a teleiophile as someone mostly sexually aroused by adults.

While the exact prevalence of pedophilia in the general population is often discussed in the scientific literature (Seto, 2007), the exact prevalence of hebephilia is rarely treated as a subject in its own right. Because of the heavy social stigma attached to pedophilia (Jahnke et al., 2013), people in surveys often hide their sexual interest in prepubescents and pubescents. In a recently published study, 99.1% of 305 male students from Canada claimed to be not even slightly sexually aroused by pubescents 12 to 14 years old (Dawson et al., 2014). This would appear to be highly inaccurate since there are many studies showing that men typically are sexually aroused by pubescents and even by prepubescents, albeit not as much as by young adults (Malamuth, 1989; Freund et al., 1970; Quinsey et al., 1975; Crèpault, 1980). In a study from 1980 cataloging erotic fantasies of men (Crèpault, 1980), 94 adult men were asked if they experienced various fantasies during intercourse or while masturbating. Sixty-six percent of men experienced the fantasy "scene where you initiate a young girl," and this fantasy was the 12th most common out of 46. Thus, many men find pubescent girls sexually exciting.

Male sexual arousal to prepubescents and pubescents is studied more reliably by phallometry. In a phallometric study a rubber ring connected to a computer is placed around the subject’s penis; the subject then views pictures of naked persons of different ages and sexes, and changes in the circumference of the erections caused by the different stimuli are measured. The results are then analyzed (e.g., erections caused by child stimuli are compared to those caused by adult stimuli). A meta-analysis of phallometric studies in general shows a high correlation (r=0.66) between subjective sexual arousal and measured genital arousal (Chivers, 2010).

In some published phallometric studies approximately 20-30% of a community sample of normal men exhibited sexual arousal to child stimuli greater than or equal to sexual arousal to adult stimuli (Firestone et al., 2000; Freund et al., 1991a). The results of these studies have been used to argue for the removal of pedophilia from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Green, 2000). But are these results typical or unusual? And what do these phallometric studies say about the proportion of men who are pedophiles and hebephiles?


2. Method

We completed a literature search in 2013 to find all phallometric studies that met these criteria:

  • Male sexual arousal to child stimuli and to adult stimuli (1) was measured;
  • Subjects were "normal" men from the community (e.g., colleges, unemployment agencies, military, etc.), and not just "sexual offenders" nor members of any other sexually defined groups that might show atypical sexual arousal patterns; (2)
  • At least 10 participants' results were reported; (3)
  • The article was published between 1970 and March 2013;
  • The article was published in English or German;
  • Data were included to determine the proportion of men more or equally sexually aroused by children (up to 13 years) than by adults. (4)

Many articles and books about pedophilia, such as Green (2002) and Norlik (2010), discuss phallometric studies of the sexual arousal in normal men to child stimuli. By searching these and other publications and by following the phallometric studies found with an initial search in Google Scholar, we found approximately 10 phallometric studies of the sexual arousal in normal men caused by child and adult stimuli. At this stage of the literature search, we identified all phallometric studies of sexual arousal in normal men by child and adult stimuli published since 1970. Some did not include the proportion of men who were more or equally sexually aroused by children than by adults. In the next step, the databases PsycInfo and SSCI were used to see which publications cited these approximately ten studies and which studies these approximately 10 publications themselves cited or described. Newly found studies were followed in the same way in the two databases. In this way, we found 26 phallometric studies of the sexual arousal of normal men by children and adults (Barsetti et al., 1998; Chaplin et al., 1995; Earls et al., 1987; Fedora et al., 1992; Firestone et al., 2000; Frenzel et al., 1989; Freund et al., 1970; Freund et al., 1972; Freund et al., 1973; Freund, 1989; Freund et al., 1991a; Freund et al., 1991b; Grossmann et al., 1992; Hall et al., 1995; Harris et al., 1992; Haywood et al., 1990; Kuban et al., 1999; Lalumière et al., 1992; Marshall et al., 1986; Marshall et al., 1988; Quinsey et al., 1975; Renaud et al., 2010; Renaud et al., 2013; Rice et al., 2012; Seto et al., 2000; Wilson, 1998).

Where possible, we e-mailed the authors of these 26 studies and asked if they knew of any additional phallometric studies; however, this endeavor did not produce any additional studies. In March 2014 we found two additional studies (Abel, 1994; Seto et al., 2001) and checked them via PsycInfo and SSCI. Of the final 28 studies, seven offered information on the number or proportion of men more or equally sexually aroused by children than by adults (Seto et al., 2001; Firestone, 2000; Hall et al., 1995; Marshall et al., 1986; Fedora et al., 1992; Freund et al., 1991a; Seto et al., 2000 (5)).

Most of the participants of the seven studies were recruited through advertisement and employment agencies, and tended to be college students, unemployed people, or men from the general community. (6)

3. Results

Table 1 shows details of the seven studies together with the information about the percentage of men more or equally sexually aroused by children up to 13 years old than by adults.

Table 1: Phallometric studies indicating the proportion of normal men more or equally sexually aroused by children (up to 13 years old) than by adults

Study 1: Seto et al. (2001): A Brief Screening Scale to Identify Pedophilic Interests Among Child Molesters - % of men who were more or equally sexually aroused by children than by adults: 17,0% - Country: Canada - Number of men: 112 - Mean age of men: 28 years (7) - Kind of Stimuli: Videoclips with audio-comment - Age of children on Stimuli: 5 to 11 years - Age of adults on Stimuli: Beginning to mid 20 Study 2: Firestone et al. (2000): Differentiation of Homicidal Child Molesters, Non-homicidal Child Molesters, and Non-offenders by Phallometry - % of men who were more or equally sexually aroused by children than by adults: 27,7% - Country: Canada - Number of men: 47 - Mean age of men: 33 years - Kind of Stimuli: Audio-stories - Age of children on Stimuli: No information - Age of adults on Stimuli: No information Study 3: Hall et al. (1995): Sexual Arousal and Arousability to Pedophilic Stimuli in a Community Sample of Normal Men - % of men who were more or equally sexually aroused by children than by adults: 29,4% (8) - Country: USA - Number of men: 80 - Mean age of men: 38 years - Kind of Stimuli: Photos and audio-stories - Age of children on Stimuli: Prepubescent girl or announcement "child below 12 years" - Age of adults on Stimuli: No information Study 4: Marshall et al. (1986): Sexual offenders against female children: Sexual preferences for age of victims and type of behavior - % of men who were more or equally sexually aroused by children than by adults: 14,3% (9) - Country: Canada - Number of men: 22 - Mean age of men: 34 years - Kind of Stimuli: Photos and audio-stories - Age of children on Stimuli: Photos: (10) 12, 13 and 14 years - Audio-Stories: Men were asked to imagine their preferred age (3- to 14- years) or a different age was chosen - Age of adults on Stimuli: 16 to 24 Years Study 5: Fedora et al. (1992): Sadism and other paraphilias in normal controls and nonaggressive sex offenders - % of men who were more or equally sexually aroused by children than by adults: 18,3% - Country: Canada - Number of men: 60 - Mean age of men: 31 years - Kind of Stimuli: Photos - Age of children on Stimuli: "Prepubescent" - Age of adults on Stimuli: No information Study 6: Freund et al. (1991a): Assessment of the sensitivity and specificity of a phallometric test: An update of "Phallometric diagnosis of pedophilia - % of men who were more or equally sexually aroused by children than by adults: 19,4% - Country: Canada - Number of men: 50 - Mean age of men: 26 years - Kind of Stimuli: Videoclips - Age of children on Stimuli: 5 to 8 years, 8 to 11 years and Pubescents (probably 12 to 13 years) - Age of adults on Stimuli: No information Study 7: Seto et al. (2000): The Discriminative Validity of a Phallometric Test for Pedophilic Interests Among Adolescent Sex Offenders Against Children - % of men who were more or equally sexually aroused by children than by adults: 25,0% (11) - Country: Canada - Number of men: 16 - Mean age of men: 20 years - Kind of Stimuli: Videoclips - Age of children on Stimuli: 5 to 13 years - Age of adults on Stimuli: Mid 20

According to the mean value of the seven phallometric studies, 21.6% of men are more or equally sexually aroused by children up to 13 years old than by adults.

The mean value of the four studies requiring greater sexual arousal to child-stimuli than to adult-stimuli to be classified as pedophilic or hebephilic (Marshall et al., 1986; Fedora et al., 1992; Freund et al., 1991a; Seto et al., 2000) was lower than that of the three studies requiring greater or equal sexual arousal (Seto et al., 2001; Firestone et al., 2000; Hall et al., 1995) (19.3% vs. 24.7%). So by this analysis, approximately one out of every five men is pedophilic or hebephilic, while one out of every four is at least as sexually aroused by children up to 13 years old as by adults.

We do not know if men who are strongly sexually aroused by children are under- or over-represented in these studies (see chapter limitations below). Philip Tromovitch guesses that pedophilic men often try to avoid taking part in such studies to hide their sexual arousal pattern (O'Carroll, 2013). If so, there could be many more men in the male population who are strongly sexually aroused by children. It is also possible that people with greater sexual arousal by children are over-represented in these studies because they are more interested in studies about sexuality. Generally, volunteers in phallometric studies tend to be more sexually experienced, have more sex partners, experience less sexual shame, and are sexually more curious (Plaud et al., 1999; Strassberg et al.1995; Wolchik et al., 1985). Although the samples of the seven studies were constructed in different ways, the results of all seven studies are in the range from 14% to 29% (14, 17, 18, 19, 25, 28 and 29%) so the 22% mean value of the seven studies seems to be roughly correct.

Approximately 40-50% of men in prison because of sexual contacts with children 14 years or younger are more or equally sexually aroused by children than by adults (Seto, 2004). Since 22% of normal men are more or equally sexually aroused by children than by adults, many more – perhaps totaling 40-50% - are quite strongly sexually aroused by children up to 13 years. So for a man to feel sexual arousal for children up to 13 years old is not at all unusual.

Percentages of pedophiles and hebephiles in the male population

Two of the seven studies allow us to roughly estimate the percentage of pedophiles and hebephiles in the male population. Seto et al. (2000) used stimuli from the age groups 8-11 (prepubertal), 12-13 (Pubescents), and mid-20’s. Marshall et al. (1986) used stimuli from the age groups 3-11 (prepubertal), 12-14 (Pubescents), and 16-24. In these two studies the sexual arousal of a total of 38 normal men was measured. One of the 38 men was mostly aroused by children below 12 years old, six by pubescents 12 to 13/14 years old, and 31 by adults (Stimuli "mid-20’s" or "16-24-year-olds"). So according to the combined results, we calculated prevalences of approximately 3% for pedophilia, 16% for hebephilia, and 82% for ephebophilia/teleiophilia. Table 2 shows the results. In the study of Seto et al. (2000) as described, no adolescent stimuli were used so it is possible that one or more of the four men who are declared in table 2 as hebephiles was in reality an ephebophile.

Table 2: Percentages of Pedophiles and Hebephiles in the male population (Seto et al. (2000) and Marshall et al. (1986))

Pedophiles: Both studies (n=38): n=1 = 3% - Seto et al., 2000 (n=16): n=0 = 0% - Marshall et al., 1986 (n=22): n=1 = 5% Hebephiles: Both studies (n=38): n=6 = 16% - Seto et al., 2000 (n=16): n=4 = 25% - Marshall et al., 1986 (n=22): n=2 = 9% Ephebophiles/Teleiophiles: Both studies (n=38): n=31 = 82% - Seto et al., 2000 (n=16): n=12 = 75% - Marshall et al., 1986 (n=22): n=19 = 86%

Another phallometric study of "sexual offenders against children" (Blanchard et al., 2009) also found a much greater prevalence of hebephilic than pedophilic response among heterosexual offenders. A national survey of Finnish adults (Alonko et. al., 2013) found sexual interest in persons 7-12 years old approximately five times more often than in persons 0-6 years old, and approximately six times more often in persons 13-15 years old than in persons 7-12 years old. So it appears that there are many more hebephilic than pedophilic men.

Most minor-attracted persons say that their age-preference already existed in their childhood or youth, and they knew about their age- preference in their childhood or youth (B4U-ACT, 2011). So not only is approximately one in five men pedophilic or hebephilic, but one in five boys is pedophilic, pre-pedophilic, hebephilic, or pre-hebephilic. (Pre- )hebephilic boys will recognize their sexual age-preference later than (pre-)pedophilic boys.


4. Conclusion

According to existing phallometric studies, approximately one in every five men is pedophilic or hebephilic. While many might find this surprising, individuals with sexual interest in children nowadays are motivated to hide it, and thus the occurrence of pedophilia and hebephilia appears to be much lower than it actually is. A similar historical phenomenon is seen regarding homosexuality: around 1860 it was believed by an expert that approximately one man out of 10,000 was homosexual (Herzer, 1987). Today we know that the figure is closer to one man out of 30, and many more are bisexual (Savin-Williams, 2006).


5. Limitations

For several reasons, we know neither the exact prevalences nor relative proportions of men’s sexual arousal to children up to 13 years old and to adults. The main reasons are:

1) All seven phallometric studies described here come from North America, and six of the seven studies come from Canada. We do not know if the level of sexual arousal caused by children is the same in other areas of the world. Since the level of homosexual teleiophilic arousal appears to be similar worldwide (Diamond, 1993; Kuyper et al., 2009; LeVay, 1996, p. 63; Seil et al., 1995), it can be surmised that the level of sexual arousal of men to children and adults is similar worldwide, too.

2) The reliability and validity of the results of phallometric studies have been criticized (Marshall, 2014; O'Donohue et al., 1992; Hanson et al., 1998; Kingston et al., 2007; McAnulty et al., 1992; McConaghy, 1989; McConaghy, 1992; Quinsey et al., 1990). The principal criticism seems not to matter here because men from the community are free to participate or not in phallometric studies, and since results of the measurements do not have consequences for their lives, there is little or no pressure to fake results. More research is needed to determine the long-term reliability/stability of phallometric diagnoses of pedophilia (Fernandez, 2002; Marshall et al., 2000; Marshall, 2014; Müller et al. 2014; Wormith, 1986), especially in men from the community. It seems there are no studies about these aspects using community samples.

3) The seven studies used different methods to calculate the rates of pedophilia and hebephilia and different kinds of stimuli. For examples, five studies used male and female stimuli (Seto et al., 2001; Firestone, 2000; Fedora et al., 1992; Freund et al., 1991a; Seto et al., 2000) but two studies just used female stimuli (Hall et al., 1995; Marshall et al., 1986). There seems to be no research on how different methods influence the measured rates of pedophilia and hebephilia. Such research is needed, too.

4) As stated above, we do not know if men who were strongly sexually aroused by children were under- or over-represented in the seven studies. Future studies should investigate whether people who take part in these studies show more or less sexual arousal to children than people who do not. In most of the seven studies men were recruited by advertisement, and it was not announced that the sexual arousal caused by children would be measured. So it does not seem likely that pedophiles or hebephiles were especially drawn to these studies. Concerning this issue the result of one study is very important: in Hall et al. (1995), 20% of the subjects said they had at least some sexual interest in children. This value is typical for many studies of the prevalence of pedophilia (Ahlers et al., 2011; Briere et al., 1989; Smiljanich et al., 1996). In the same study, 29% of men were more or equally sexually aroused by child stimuli then by adult stimuli. This is a strong indication that the mean value of 22% is not the result of a simple over-represenation of men who are strongly sexually aroused by children.

5) Only in September 2014 after writing this article the author learned, by reading a publication of Marshall (1997), that in the phallometric study of Marshall et al. (1986) men were screened to exclude any who reported feeling attracted to children. Still in this study 3 of the 22 men (14 %) were more sexually aroused by children than by adults. Maybe without this screening the amount of men more sexually aroused by children than by adults in this study would have been higher. Judging on the basis of the results of the other studies it is most likely that in the study of Marshall et al. (1986) because of the screening one pedophilic or one hebephilic man was excluded from the measurement. According to all seven studies including the study of Marshall et al. (1986) 22 % of men are more or equally sexually aroused by children then by adults, according to the six studies without the study of Marshall et al. (1986) 23 % of men are more or equally sexually aroused by children than by adults. So the influence of the study of Marshall et al. (1986) onto the results of this meta-analysis is small. Because of this and also to make it possible to roughly estimate the percentages of pedophiles and hebephiles in the male population with the help of the studies of Seto et al. (2000) and Marshall et al. (1986), the study of Marshall et al. (1986) was not removed afterwards.


6. Discussion

The results of this literature study have several implications:

1) More research is needed to determine the proportions of men who are pedophilic, hebephilic, ephebophilic, and teleiophilic. This author is not aware of a single scientific study that attempts to quantify all of them; indeed, he cannot find any serious discussion of merely the proportion of men who are hebephilic. It seems irresponsible to propose to make hebephilia a mental disorder without first determining this proportion. If 20% of men are hebephilic, does that sexual age-preference realistically constitute a psychiatric illness? Being diagnosed with the "mental illness” of hebephilia, no matter how defined, would damage the psychological well-being of a large portion of the male population. Scientists and especially psychologists should be aware of the deleterious influence their work can have.

2) Many pedophilic and hebephilic persons have suicidal thoughts because of their age-preference. Some of them attempt suicide, and still others accomplish it. These phenomena often occur in the age range 14- 20 years old (B4U-ACT, 2011). A large contributor to this problem may be that pedophilic and hebephilic boys and men do not talk with others about their sexual age-preference. More research should be done to find out what percentage of suicide attempts is related to sexual age- preference. According to a non-representative study about 13 percent of minor-attracted-persons attempt suicide during their life because of problems with their sexual age preference (B4U-ACT, 2011). According to an estimate of the World Health Organization generally 10 to 20 suicide attempts lead to one death (World Health Organization, 1999). If every fifth teenager and man is pedophilic or hebephilic this could mean that in the next decades worldwide millions of pedophilic and hebephilic teenagers and men will kill themselves if the stigmatization and criminalization of pedophilia and hebephilia is not stopped.

3) Most lay people know the word pedophilia but do not know the word hebephilia. Many boys and men who are minor-attracted consider themselves pedophilic but are actually hebephilic. Hebephilia has never been a mental disorder nor paraphilia in the "Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders" (Frances et al., 2011). Allen Frances and Michael B. First, chairmen of the workgroup of the DSM IV, wrote in 2011: "Hebephilia is not a paraphilia, because the sexual arousal pattern that would define it is not inherently deviant. Normal men have fantasies and urges in response to pubescent targets...The alleged diagnosis paraphilia not otherwise specified, hebephilia, arose, not out of psychiatry, but rather to meet a perceived need in the correctional system. This solution represents a misuse of the diagnostic system and of psychiatry." (Frances et al., 2011, p. 84, emphasis added) Boys and men should be informed in schools and through mass media that:

  • to be mostly sexually aroused by pubescents (11-14 years old) is widespread and not considered a mental disorder by the American Psychiatric Association;
  • important and recognized scientists like Richard Green consider pedophilia, like hebephilia, not to be a mental disorder because pedophilia, like hebephilia, is widespread and is not inherently combined with harm (Green, 2002); and
  • nobody chooses his sexual age-preference and that everybody has the right to be respected and to be loved no matter what sexual age-preference he or she has.

4) Since approximately one in five boys and men is pedophilic or hebephilic, the general stigmatization and criminalization of pedophilia and hebephilia should be reconsidered in this light. There have been many children documented to love and desire adults sexually (Rivas, 2013; Kilpatrick, 1992; Rind et al., 2014; Rind, 2013; Rind, 2003). There is not a single scientific study showing that children only love and desire children and do not love and desire adults. The love and consensual sexuality between children and adults are normal and according to many studies not harmful (Rind et al., 1998). Younger children are not harmed more than older children because of sexual relationships with adults (Finkelhor, 1979; Rind et al., 1998). The amount of the age discrepancy between the sexual partners does not significantly influence the sexual enjoyment or the positive or negative results of a relationship of a child with an adult (Kilpatrick, 1992; Rind et al., 2014; Leitenberg et al., 2003). Of course children should be well and specifically protected (Rivas, 2013), but pedophilic and hebephilic boys are children, too, and also have a right to protection. To stigmatize pedophilic or hebephilic boys as mentally ill and to put them in prison for their love are detrimental to their mental health. Only unethical sexual behavior should be punished (Rivas, 2013). One in every five boys and men is pedophilic or hebephilic, and this new insight deserves a human and compassionate response from society.

Footnotes

(1) The study of Quinsey et al. (1988) was not included in the analysis because the stimuli described only forced sex between children and adults.

(2) The study of Lang et al. (1988) was not included in the analysis because the researchers excluded men with a sexual interest in children.

(3) The study of Quinsey et al. (1976) was not included because it had only five participants, which means that one men had the value of 20 percent which could distort the results of this study.

(4) The age 13 was choosen because in Germany children are defined by law as people under 14 years old. This definition accords with popular use of the word children.

(5) It is possible that the 16 men of the study of Seto et al. (2000) were partly or completely included among the 50 men of the study of Freund et al. (1991a). Seto e-mailed us on July 15, 2013 to say that without the data of the study from Freund et al. (1991a) he cannot determine if there is an overlap.

(6) The relevant passages of the seven studies are: "(...) the majority were recruited from a local community college." (Seto et al., 2001); "The comparison group was recruited through an advertisement and paid a $50 honorarium." (Firestone et al., 2000); "Subjects were recruited via a newspaper advertisement placed in the sports section of the Sunday edition of a newspaper in a large city on two occasions. The newspaper advertisement read as follows: Earn $40 for participation in an experiment on human sexuality that will require a total of 8 hours on two consecutive days. Males 18 and older only." (Hall et al., 1995); "Twenty- two normal controls were recruited from local employment agencies or from local training programs designed for the chronically unemployed." (Marshall et al., 1986); "Normal controls consisted of 66 paid males recruited from among hospital staff and the community at large." (Fedora et al., 1992); "The (...) group consisted of 50 paid volunteers. These men were recruited from a government placement office for the unemployed and from community colleges (...)" (Freund et al., 1991a); "Non-offender participants were recruited between 1985 and 1990 through advertisements posted around a community college campus and were paid $15 an hour for their time." (Seto et al., 2000).

(7) 27,6 years is the mean age of the complete control group of the study consisting of 112 men from the community and 94 "rapists".

(8) 29,4% is the mean value of the arousal through pictures (32,5%) and audio-stories (26,25%).

(9) In this study three of the 22 men (14,3%) were more or equally sexually aroused by 12-, 13- and 14-year olds than by adults and one man (4,5%) who was more or equally sexually aroused by 3- to 11-year olds than by adults. The first number 14,3% was used here although the stimuli included 14-year-olds because this value seems to be more relevant for the main question of this article (sexual aroused by children up to 13 years old).

(10) In this study the men also saw pictures of children from the age of 3 to 11 years old. The mean value of the arousal caused by these pictures was compared to the arousal caused by pictures of adults, so this mean value was not helpful for the research question of this article (sexual aroused by children up to 13 years old) and was not used here. (11) In table 2 (Seto et al. (2000) p. 325) it is written that 3 of 16 men were mostly aroused by 12- to 13-year-old girls and that one man was mostly aroused by 12- to 13-year-old boys.

Literature

Abel, G. G. et al. (1994): SCREENING TESTS FOR PEDOPHILIA. Criminal Justice and Behavior, Volume 21, Issue 1, March 1994, pp. 115-132

Ahlers, C. J. et al. (2011): How unusual are the contents of paraphilias? Journal of Sexual Medicine, Volume 8, Issue 5, pp. 1362-1370

Alanko, K., B. Salo, A. Mokros and P. Santtila (2013): Evidence for heritability of adult men's sexual interest in youth under age 16 from a population-based extended twin design. J Sex Med., April 2013, Volume 10, Issue 4, pp. 1090-1099

Barsetti, Ian, Christopher M. Earls, Martin L. Lalumière and Nathalie Bélanger (1998): The Differentiation of Intrafamilial and Extrafamilial Heterosexual Child Molesters. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, Volume 13, Issue 2, pp. 275-286

B4U-ACT (2011): "Spring 2011 Survey Results". http://b4uact.org/science/survey/01.htm (accessed March 25, 2014)

Blanchard, Ray, Amy D. Lykins, Diane Wherrett, Michael E. Kuban, James M. Cantor, Thomas Blak, Robert Dickey and Philip E. Klassen (2009): Pedophilia, Hebephilia, and the DSM-V. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 38, pp. 335-350

Briere, John and Marsha Runtz (1989): University males’ sexual interest in children: Predicting potential indices of “pedophilia” in a non-forensic sample. Child Abuse and Neglect, Volume 13, pp. 65-75

Cantor, James et al. (2005): Handedness in pedophilia and hebephilia. Arch Sex Behav, August 2005, Volume 34, Issue 4, pp. 447-459

Chaplin, Terry, Marnie E. Rice and Grant T. Harris (1995): Salient Victim Suffering and the Sexual Responses of Child Molesters. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Volume 63, Issue 2, pp. 249-255

Chivers, Meredith L. (2010): A brief update on the specificity of sexual arousal. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, Volume 25, Issue 4, pp. 407- 414

Crèpault, Claude et. al. (1980) : Men´s erotic fantasies. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 9, Issue 6, pp. 565-581

Dawson, Samantha J., Brittany A. Bannerman and Martin L. Lalumière (2014): Paraphilic Interests: An Examination of Sex Differences in a Nonclinical Sample. Sex Abuse, published online March 14th 2014 Diamond, Milton (1993): Homosexuality and Bisexuality in Different Populations. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 22, Issue 4, pp. 291- 310

Earls, Christopher M., Vernon L. Quinsey and Louis Georges Castonguay (1987): A Comparison of Three Methods of Scoring Penile Circumference Changes. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 16, Issue 6, pp. 493-500

Fedora, O., J. R. Reddon, J. W. Morrison, S. K. Fedora, H. Pascoe and L. T. Yeudall (1992): Sadism and other paraphilias in normal controls and nonaggressive sex offenders. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 21, pp. 1–15

Fernandez, Yolanda (2002): Phallometric testing with sexual offenders against female victims: Limits to its value. Forum on Corrections Research , Volume 14, Issue 1, 2002 January, pp. 7-12

Finkelhor, David (1979): Sexually Victimized Children. The Free Press, New York

Firestone, Philip, John M. Bradford, David M. Greenberg and Kevin L. Nunes (2000): Differentiation of Homicidal Child Molesters, Nonhomicidal Child Molesters, and Nonoffenders by Phallometry. American Journal of Psychology, Volume 157, pp. 1847-1850

Frances, Allen and Michael B. First (2011): Hebephilia Is Not a Mental Disorder in DSM-IV-TR and Should Not Become One in DSM-5. J Am Acad Psychiatry Law, Volume 39, Issue 1, February 2011, pp. 78-85

Frenzel, Roy R. and Reuben A. Lang (1989): Identifying sexual preferences in intrafamilial and extrafamilial child sexual abusers. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, Volume 2, Issue 3, pp. 255-275

Freund, Kurt and R. Costell (1970): The structure of erotic preference in the nondeviant male. Behavior Research and Therapy, Volume 88, pp. 15-20.

Freund, Kurt, C. K. McKnight, R. Langevin and S. Cibiri (1972): The female child as a surrogate object. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 2, Issue 2, pp. 119-133

Freund, Kurt, R. Langevin, S. Cibiri and Y. Zajac (1973): Heterosexual aversion in homosexual males. British Journal of Psychiatry, Volume 122, pp. 163-169

Freund, Kurt and Ray Blanchard (1989): Phallometric Diagnosis of Pedophilia. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Volume 57, Issue 1, 100-105

Freund, K. and R. Watson (1991a): Assessment of the sensitivity and specificity of a phallometric test: An update of "Phallometric diagnosis of pedophilia. Psychological Assessment, Volume 3, pp. 254-260

Freund, Kurt, Robin Watson, Robert Dickey and Douglas Rienzo (1991b): Erotic Gender Differentiation in Pedophilia. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 20, Issue 6, pp. 555-566

Green, Richard (2002): Is Pedophilia a Mental Disorder? Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 31 , Issue 6, pp. 479-481

Grossman, Linda, James L. Jr. Cavanaugh and Thomas W. Haywood (1992): Deviant sexual responsiveness on penile plethysmography using visual stimuli: alleged child molesters vs. normal control subjects. J Nerv Ment Dis. March 1992, Volume 180, Issue 3, pp. 207-208.

Haas-Rietschel, Helga (2013): „Kein Täter werden“. Published January 10th 2013, https://www.gew.de/Kein_Taeter_werden.html (looked at March 16th 2014)

Hall, Gordon H., Richard Hirschman and Lori L. Oliver (1995): Sexual Arousal and Arousability to Pedophilic Stimuli in a Community Sample of Normal Men. Behavior Therapy, Volume 26, pp. 681-694

Hanson, R. Karl and Monique T. Bussiere (1998): Predicting Relapse: A Meta-Analysis of Sexual Offender Recidivism Studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Volume 66, Issue 2, pp. 348-362

Harris, Grant T., Marnie E. Rice, Vernon L. Quinsey, Terry C. Chaplin and Christopher Earls (1992): Maximizing the Discriminant Validity of Phallometric Data. Psychological Assessment, Volume 4, Issue 4, pp. 502-511

Haywood, Thomas W., Linda S. Grossman and James L. Cavanaugh (1990): Subjective versus objective measurements of deviant sexual arousal in clinical evaluations of alleged child molesters. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Volume 2, Issue 2, pp. 269-275

Herzer, Manfred (1987): Schwule Preußen, warme Berliner. Überarbeitete Fassung eines Vortrages, http://www.datenkerker.de/_texte/homosexualitaet.geschichte.berlin.pdf (looked at February 10th 2013)

Jahnke, Sara and Jürgen Hoyer (2013): Stigmatization of People With Pedophilia: A Blind Spot in Stigma Research. International Journal of Sexual Health, Volume 25, pp. 169–184

Kilpatrick, Allie C. (1992): Long-Range Effects of Child and Adolescent Sexual Experiences. Myths, Mores, Menaces. Lawrence Erlbaum Assiciates, Publishers, Hillsdale

Kingston, Drew A. , Philip Firestone, Heather M. Moulden and John M. Bradford (2007): The Utility of the Diagnosis of Pedophilia: A Comparison of Various Classification Procedures. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 36, pp. 423-436

Kuban, Michael, Howard E. Barbaree and Ray Blanchard (1999): A Comparison of Volume and Circumference Phallometry: Response Magnitude and Method Agreement. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 28, Issue 4, pp. 345-359

Kuyper, Lisette and Ine Vanwesenbeeck (2009): High Levels of Same- Sex Experiences in the Netherlands: Prevalences of Same-Sex Experiences in Historical and International Perspective. Journal of Homosexuality, Volume 56, Issue 8, pp. 993-1010

Lalumière, Martin L. and Christopher M. Earls (1992): Voluntary Control of Penile Responses as a Function of Stimulus Duration and Instructions. Behavioral Assessment, Volume 14, pp. 121-132

Lang, Reuben A., Edward L. Black, Roy R. Frenzel and Kenneth L. Checkley (1988): Aggression and erotic attraction toward children in incestuous and pedophilic men. Annals of sex research, Volume 1, Issue 3, pp. 417-441

Laws, D. Richard and William O´Donohue (1997): Sexual Deviance. Theory, Assessment, and Treatment. THE GUILFORD PRESS, New York, London

Leitenberg, Harold and Heidi Saltzman (2003): College women who had sexual intercourse when they were underage minors (13-15): age of their male partners, relation to current adjustment, and statutory rape implications. Sex Abuse, April, Volume 15, Issue 2, pp. 135-147

Malamuth, Neil M. (1989): The Attraction to Sexual Aggression scale: Part I. Journal of Sex Research, Volume 26, pp. 26-49

Marshall, W. L. , H. E. Barbaree and D. Christophe (1986): Sexual offenders against female children: Sexual preferences for age of victims and type of behaviour. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement, Volume 18, Issue 4, pp. 424- 439

Marshall, W.L. , H.E. Barbaree and Jennifer Butt (1988): Sexual offenders against male children: Sexual preferences. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Volume 26, Issue 5, pp. 383-391

Marshall, W. L. (1997): PEDOPHILIA: Psychopathology and Theory. In: Laws et al. (1997), pp. 152-174

Marshall, W. L. and Y. Fernandez (2000): Phallometric testing with sexual offenders: limits to its value. Clin Psychol Rev, October 2000, 20 (7) pp. 807-822

Marshall, W. L. (2014): Phallometric assessments of sexual interests: an update. Curr Psychiatry Rep, 2014 January, 16 (1) 428

McAnulty, Richard D. and Henry E. Adams (1992): Validity and Ethics of Penile Circumference Measures of Sexual Arousal: A Reply to McConaghy. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 21, Issue 9, pp. 177- 186

McConaghy, Nathaniel (1989): Validity and Ethics of Penile Circumference Measures of Sexual Arousal: A Critical Review. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 18, Issue 4, pp. 357-369

McConaghy, Nathaniel (1992): Validity and Ethics of Penile Circumference Measures of Sexual Arousal: A Response to McAnulty and Adams. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 21, Issue 2, pp. 187-195

Müller, Karolina, Susan Curry, Rebekah Ranger, Peer Briken, John Bradford and John Paul Fedoroff (2014): Changes in Sexual Arousal as Measured by Penile Plethysmography in Men with Pedophilic Sexual Interest. The Journal of Sexual Medicine, published online March 17th 2014

Norlik, Mark (2010): Kein Pardon für solche Liebe. Vom Lieben und Leiden einer Minderheit. (PDF-file, see http://www.itp-arcados.net/buch- kein-pardon-mark-norlik.php)

O'Carroll, Tom (2013): Tromovitch sets a poser on prevalence. Published July 19th 2013. http://tomocarroll.wordpress.com/2013/07/19/tromovitch-sets-a-poser- on-prevalence/

O'Donohue, William and Elizabeth Letourneau (1992): The Psychometric Properties of the Penile Tumescence Assessment of Child Molesters. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, Volume 14, Issue 12, pp. 123-174

Plaud, Joseph J. , George A. Gaither, Holly J. Hegstad, Leslie Rowan and Mary K. Devitt (1999): Volunteer bias in human psychophysiological sexual arousal research: To Whom do our research results apply? Journal of Sex Research, Volume 36, Issue 2, pp. 171-179

Quinsey, Vernon L. , Cary M. Steinman, Sidney G. Bergersen and Timothy Holmnes (1975): Penile Circumference, Skin Conductance, and Ranking Responses of Child Molesters and "Normals" to Sexual and Nonsexual Visual Stimuli. Behavior Therapy, Volume 6, Issue 2, pp. 213- 219

Quinsey, Vernon L. and Sidney G. Bergersen (1976): Instructional control of penile circumference in assessments of sexual preference. Behavior Therapy, Volume 7, Issue 4, July 1976, pp. 489–493

Quinsey, Vernon L. and Terry C. Chaplin (1988): Penile Responses of Child Molesters and Normals to Descriptions of Encounters with Children Involving Sex and Violence. J Interpers Violence, September 1988, Volume 3, Issue 3, pp. 259-274

Quinsey, Vernon L. and D. R. Laws (1990): Validity of Physiological Measures of Pedophilic Sexual Arousal in a Sexual Offender Population: A Critique of Hall, Proctor, and Nelson. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Volume 6, pp. 886-888

Renaud, Patrice, Joanne-L. Rouleau, Jean Proulx et al. (2010): Virtual characters designed for forensic assessment and rehabilitation of sex offenders: standardized and made-to-measure. Journal of Virtual Reality and Broadcasting, Volume 7, Issue 5,

Renauld, Patrice, Sylvain Chartier, Joanne-Lucine Rouleau u.a. (2013): Using immersive virtual reality and ecological psychology to probe into child molesters' phenomenology. Journal of Sexual Aggression, Volume 19, Issue 1, pp. 102-120

Rice, Marnie E., Grant T. Harris, Carol Lang and Terry C. Chaplin (2012): Adolescents Who Have Sexually Offended : Is Phallometry Valid? Sexual Abuse, Volume 24, Issue 2, pp. 133-152

Rind, Bruce, Robert Bauserman and Philip Tromovitch (1998): A meta- analytic examination of assumed properties of child sexual abuse using college samples. Psychological Bulletin, American Psychological Association, Volume 124, Issue 1, pp. 22-53

Rind, Bruce (2003): Adolescent Sexual Experiences with Adults: Pathological or Functional? Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, Volume 15, Issue 1, pp. 5-22

Rind, Bruce (2010): Social Response to Age-Gap Sex Involving Minors: Empirical, Historical, Cross-Cultural, and Cross-Species Considerations. Journal of Boyhood Studies, Volume 4, Issue 2, pp. 113-125

Rind, Bruce (2013): Homosexual Orientation - From Nature, Not Abuse: A Critique of Roberts, Glymour, and Koenen (2013). Archives of Sexual Behavior. Published online: 22 March 2013

Rind, Bruce and Max Welter (2014): Enjoyment and Emotionally Negative Reactions in Minor–Adult Versus Minor–Peer and Adult–Adult First Postpubescent Coitus: A Secondary Analysis of the Kinsey Data. Arch Sex Behav, Volume 43, Issue 2, pp. 285-297

Rivas, T. (2013): Positive Memories. Cases of positive memories of erotic and platonic relationships and contacts of children with adults, as seen from the perspective of the former minor. Copyright 2012 by T. Rivas. 1st print, November 2012, edited by Ipce, http://www.ipce.info/host/rivas/positive_memories.htm

Savin-Williams, Ritch C. (2006): Who's Gay? Does It Matter? Current Directions in Psychological Science, Volume 15, Issue 1, pp. 40-44

Seil, Randall L., James A. Wells and David Wypij (1995): The Prevalence of Homosexual Behavior and Attraction in the United States, the United Kingdom and France: Results of National Population-Based Samples. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 24, Issue 3, pp. 235-248

Seto, Michael, Martin L. Lalumière and Ray Blanchard (2000): The Discriminating Validity of a Phallometric Test for Pedophilic Interests Among Adolescent Sex Offenders Against Children. Psychological Assessmnent, Volume 12, Issue 3, pp. 319-327

Seto, Michael C. and Martin L. Lalumière (2001): A Brief Screening Scale to Identify Pedophilic Interests Among Child Molesters. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, Volume 13, Issue 1, pp. 15-25 Seto, Michael C. (2004): Pedophilia and sexual offenses against children. Annual Review of Sex Research, Volume 15, Issue 1, pp. 321-361.

Seto, Michael C. (2007): Pedophilia and Sexual Offending Against Children: Theory, Assessment, and Intervention. Amer Psychological Assn Smiljanich, Kathy and John Briere (1996): Self-Reported Sexual Interest in Children: Sex Differences and Psychosocial Correlates in a University Sample. Violence and Victims, Volume 11, Issue 1, pp. 39-50

Strassberg, Donald S. and Kristi Lowe (1995): Volunteer Bias in Sexuality Research. Archives of Sexual Behavior, Volume 24, Issue 4, pp. 369-382

Wilson, Robin J. (1998): Psychophysiological Signs of Faking in the Phallometric Test. Sexual Abuse, Volume 10, Issue 2, pp. 113-126

Wolchik, Sharlene A., Sanford L. Braver and Karen Jensen (1985): Volunteer bias in erotica research: Effects of intrusiveness of measure and sexual background. Archives of Sexual Behavior, April 1985, Volume 14, Issue 2, pp. 93-107

World Health Organization (Department of Mental Health Geneva) (1999): FIGURES & FACTS ABOUT SUICIDE, http://whqlibdoc.who.int/hq/1999/WHO_MNH_MBD_99.1.pdf

Wormith, J. S. (1986): Assessing deviant sexual arousal: Psychological and cognitive aspects. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, Volume 8, Issue 3, pp. 101-137


September 23, 2014

Creative Commons by-nd